9.2 Sexual Selection
Darwin noticed that there were many traits and behaviors of organisms that could not be explained by natural adaptation or “survival value.” For example, the brilliant plumage of peacocks should actually lower their rates of survival. That is, the peacocks’ feathers act like a neon sign to predators, advertising “Easy, delicious dinner here!” But if these bright feathers lower peacocks’ chances at survival, why have them? The same can be asked of similar characteristics of other animals, such as the large antlers of male stags or the wattles of roosters, which also seem to be unfavorable to survival. Again, if these traits make the animals less likely to survive, why did they develop in the first place? And how have these animals continued to survive with these traits over thousands and thousands of years? Darwin’s answer to this conundrum was the theory of sexual selection: the evolution of characteristics, not because of survival advantage, but because of mating advantage.
Sexual selection occurs through two processes. The first, intrasexual competition, occurs when members of one sex compete against each other, and the winner gets to mate with a member of the opposite sex. Male stags, for example, battle with their antlers, and the winner (often the stronger one with larger antlers) gains mating access to the female. That is, even though large antlers make it harder for the stags to run through the forest and evade predators (which lowers their survival success), more impressive antlers provide the stags with a better chance of attracting a mate (which increases their reproductive success).
Human males sometimes also compete against each other in physical contests: boxing, wrestling, karate, or group-on-group sports, such as football. Even though engaging in these activities poses a “threat” to their safety or survival success, as with the stag, the victors are often more attractive to potential mates, increasing their reproductive success. Thus, whatever qualities lead to success in intrasexual competition are more likely to be passed on with greater frequency due to their association with greater mating success.
The second process of sexual selection is preferential mate choice, also called intersexual selection. In this process, if members of one sex are attracted to certain qualities in mates— such as brilliant plumage, signs of good health, or even intelligence—those desired qualities get passed on in greater numbers, simply because those individuals mate more often. For example, the colorful plumage of peacocks exists due to a long evolutionary history of peahens’ (the term for female peacocks) attraction to males with brilliantly colored feathers. In all sexually reproducing species, adaptations in both sexes (males and females) exist due to survival selection and sexual selection. That is, natural adaptation and sexual selection are not mutually exclusive. However, unlike other animals where one sex has dominant control over mate choice, humans have “mutual mate choice.” That is, both women and men typically have a say in choosing their mates. And both mates value qualities such as kindness, intelligence, and dependability that are beneficial to long-term relationships—qualities that make good partners and good parents.
Removing the Male Bias
In 1871, Charles Darwin (1809-1882) wrote about sexual selection in his book, The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. Though he outlined many of the underlying processes, there was one primary issue – the timing of his work. The Victorian Era was marked by extreme sex differences in which men were viewed as strong, competitive, and intellectual and women were viewed as docile, domestic, and restrained. Because of this, research into sex differences, particularly when investigating mate selection and choice, spent decades impaired by this bias. This perspective marked the scientific investigation of mating and mate selection so completely that species with promiscuous or aggressive females were often discounted as abhorrent exceptions. In essence, like Victorian women, females of other species were viewed as passive actors in the mating game.
It wasn’t until the late 20th century that females were truly studied as players in the game. From birds to spiders, bats to bonobos, scholars sought to understand the role of female choice. Zoologist Lucy Cooke‘s newest book, Bitch: On the Female of the Species, explores the history, research, and importance of studying females in their own right.
a mode of natural selection in which members of one biological sex choose mates of the other sex to mate with (intersexual selection), and compete with members of the same sex for access to members of the opposite sex (intrasexual selection).
competition between members of the same sex (usually males) for access to mates.
a measure of an individual's production of offspring per breeding event or lifetime.
any pattern of behavior shown by members of one sex that leads to preferred mating with certain members of the opposite sex.
Members of the competitive sex show off for mates and the opposite sex chooses the best display.